少点错误 14小时前
Making Sense of Consciousness Part 4: States of Consciousness
index_new5.html
../../../zaker_core/zaker_tpl_static/wap/tpl_guoji1.html

 

文章探讨了丘脑在意识状态中的关键作用,指出在无意识或意识减弱状态下,丘脑的活跃度降低,与其他脑区的功能连接减弱。文章分析了丘脑在植物人状态、失神发作、麻醉和深度睡眠中的表现,强调丘脑是整合不同感觉感知和皮质处理区域的连接枢纽,对理解意识机制至关重要。

🔍丘脑在无意识或意识减弱状态下活跃度降低,倾向于慢同步放电而非快去同步放电,且与皮质等功能连接减弱。

📊研究发现,植物人状态患者80%存在丘脑损伤,失神发作和植物人状态患者对疼痛刺激的脑活动仅限于脑干、丘脑及初级感觉和听觉皮层。

🌜丘脑在深度睡眠中起主导作用,通过T型钙通道引导慢睡眠纺锤波振荡,其慢波放电模式先于皮质出现。

🔌丘脑是连接不同感觉感知和皮质处理区域的枢纽,可能是意识机制的关键区域,但刺激丘脑治疗昏迷效果尚不明确。

🧬皮质较灵活,而丘脑等皮质下结构更固定,丘脑可能编码更基本的意识功能,对理解意识机制更具潜力。

Published on August 8, 2025 9:21 PM GMT

the red arrow points to the thalamus

In the past 3 entries in my consciousness sequence I was focused on conscious sensory perception — the difference between sense data that we subjectively “notice” or “are aware of”, vs. sense data that we are exposed to, and which may affect us, but without our knowledge.

This is one of Erik Hoel’s three common-sense characteristics of consciousness. We are conscious of a thing when we have a subjective experience of noticing or perceiving it; we are unconscious of the same thing when we have no such experience.

Another common-sense characteristic of consciousness is awakeness (or “arousal”). Ordinary waking life is a conscious state; dreamless sleep, anaesthesia, and coma are unconscious states.

In unconscious states, the individual is largely unresponsive to outside stimuli, cannot communicate, and will wake with no memories of having experienced anything while unconscious.

As far as we can tell, in an unconscious state, the individual has no conscious/subjective perceptions of anything. If being conscious “of something” means that it enters a sort of “workspace” or “field of awareness” where we can notice/perceive it, then an unconscious state means an empty workspace or field of awareness.

So, what’s different in the brain during an unconscious or less-conscious state?

At a first approximation, it’s the thalamus, stupid.

During less-conscious states, the thalamus is less active, more inclined to slow synchronized firing than fast desynchronized firing, and less “functionally connected to” (i.e. activity correlates less well with) other brain regions, particularly in the cortex.

Interestingly, my last (imperfect) candidate for a brain region critical for conscious sensory perception was a particular nucleus in the thalamus.

Neural Correlates of Disorders of Consciousness: It’s The Thalamus (and Friends)

“Disorders of Consciousness” (DOC) is a catch-all term that includes things like coma, minimally conscious states, vegetative states, and so on.

Interestingly, there are two axes on which disorders of consciousness differ from the normal healthy waking state: arousal and awareness.1

In a vegetative state the patient is unresponsive to their surroundings, but still has open eyes and sleep-wake cycles. They are, in some sense, awake…but appear to not be aware of anything around them. (Typically, a coma that doesn’t end earlier will turn into a vegetative state after a few weeks.)

Similarly, in an absence seizure, some degree of wakefulness is still present (open eyes, standing, etc) but no response to their surroundings. In both absence seizures and vegetative states, patients typically have no memory of the experience after they recover, consistent with them having no conscious subjective experiences during the “low awareness state.”

While lesions in many places can cause disorders of consciousness, the thalamus is a common lesion site, as well as a network of fronto-parietal cortex sites known as the default mode network (which is typically active during self-related thought processes like mind-wandering and autobiographical memory.)

In a meta-analysis of fMRI studies of disorders of consciousness, covering a total of 687 patients and 637 healthy controls, activity in the DOC patients was significantly reduced in regions within the default mode network, including2:

A subsequent study similarly found that clinical measures of awareness, in disorders of consciousness, correlated with the degree of damage to the thalamus.3

The vegetative state, in particular, is overwhelmingly likely to coincide with damage to the thalamus (80% of patients).4

Interestingly, you can get brain activity in response to painful stimuli and sounds in vegetative state patients — but only in brainstem, thalamus, and the “primary” somatosensory and auditory cortices, not in the “secondary” sensory-processing areas that typically correlate with conscious sensory perception and more complex pattern-recognition.5

From an EEG point of view, patients in comas or vegetative states usually have only very slow-wave activity, delta or theta waves, similar to deep sleep. Brain-dead patients have no EEG activity; “locked-in” patients, who are conscious and can communicate by thought alone, but can’t move, have normal EEGs.6

Neural Correlates of Anaesthesia: Yep, Still The Thalamus

During propofol anaesthesia, EEG waves slow down and increase in amplitude, first to alpha waves, and then to delta and theta waves.7

Under PET imaging, lots of regions in the brain show lower metabolism and blood flow during anaesthesia, but particularly the thalamus and cortex.8

You get the same results with fMRI imaging: reliably, less blood flow to the thalamus, with a strong dose-response relationship — the more anaesthetic, the less thalamic blood flow. 9

Neural Correlates of Deep Sleep: Once Again, That’s The Thalamus

The classic neural correlate of sleep is the EEG.

Beta waves for alert (or dreaming) states, alpha waves for calm or drowsy states, and delta and theta waves for deep sleep.

These are brain-wide patterns of electrical activity, averaged across the whole brain volume and measured with scalp electrodes.

But there’s some evidence that the thalamus leads the whole-brain transition from fast uncorrelated spiking (and thus high-frequency EEG patterns) to slower and more synchronized brain waves (and thus the lower-frequency delta and theta waves.)

During the normal waking state, neurons in the thalamus spike “tonically” (at a sustained, high baseline rate). By contrast, during sleep, the thalamus “sets the pace” for the slow “sleep spindle” oscillations characteristic of entry into deep non-REM sleep. It’s actually T-type calcium channels in the thalamus, specifically, that entrain these sleep spindles, and you can induce them artificially in rodents with optogenetics.10 Both sleep spindles and slow-wave oscillations in sleeping humans happen first in thalamus and only afterwards in the cortex.11

Other regions, like the brainstem and hypothalamus, are also highly involved in the transition between sleep and waking. You can induce sleep in a cat with pulsed electrode stimulation to lots of parts of the brain, including the basal forebrain12 , the cortex13, and the thalamus14.

But, for instance, there are particular neurons in the thalamus active during the process of loud sounds awaking animals from deep sleep, and optogenetic stimulation of those neurons will wake the animal up.15

It seems that “lower” regions like the hypothalamus and brainstem connect to the thalamus, and have some of their effects on sleep through the thalamus. So while it’s not the only region important for sleep, it might be the proximate controller of sleep and slow-wave firing patterns in the rest of the brain.

Upshots

This is a very high-level overview; we haven’t touched on where or how the thalamus affects states of consciousness, or any DOC-adjacent conditions like confusional states.

But we do have a very clear and consistent signal that virtually all unconscious states involve alterations in the thalamus, which is intriguing because the thalamus is also the connecting hub that integrates between different sensory perceptions and the cortical regions that process them.

Unfortunately, we don’t (yet) seem to have reliable results at bringing people out of comatose states by stimulating the thalamus1617.

But we might learn more about how consciousness works by zooming in on this region. As a rough heuristic, the (human) cortex tends to be very flexible, while subcortical brain structures are less so; if any function is going to be anatomically “hard-coded”, it’ll probably be subcortical. It’s also more likely that more “ancient” subcortical structures will be similar between humans and animals. So if the toolbox of neuroscience is to be helpful for making sense of consciousness, we should be more hopeful about subcortical sites. And luckily we seem to have one!

1

Di Perri, Carol, et al. "Functional neuroanatomy of disorders of consciousness." Epilepsy & Behavior 30 (2014): 28-32.

2

Hannawi, Yousef, et al. "Resting brain activity in disorders of consciousness: a systematic review and meta-analysis." Neurology 84.12 (2015): 1272-1280.

3

Lutkenhoff, Evan S., et al. "Thalamic and extrathalamic mechanisms of consciousness after severe brain injury." Annals of neurology 78.1 (2015): 68-76.

4

Adams, J. Hume, D. I. Graham, and Bryan Jennett. "The neuropathology of the vegetative state after an acute brain insult." Brain 123.7 (2000): 1327-1338.

5

Laureys, Steven. "The neural correlate of (un) awareness: lessons from the vegetative state." Trends in cognitive sciences 9.12 (2005): 556-559.

6

Bernat, James L. "Chronic disorders of consciousness." The Lancet 367.9517 (2006): 1181-1192.

7

Hagihira, S. "Changes in the electroencephalogram during anaesthesia and their physiological basis." British journal of anaesthesia 115.suppl_1 (2015): i27-i31.

8

Menon, D. M. "Editorial I: Mapping the anatomy of unconsciousness—imaging anaesthetic action in the brain." British Journal of Anaesthesia 86.5 (2001): 607-617.

9

Song, Xiao-xing, and Bu-wei Yu. "Anesthetic effects of propofol in the healthy human brain: functional imaging evidence." Journal of anesthesia 29.2 (2015): 279-288.

10

Whyte, Christopher J., et al. "Thalamic contributions to the state and contents of consciousness." Neuron 112.10 (2024): 1611-1625.

11

Schreiner, Thomas, et al. "The human thalamus orchestrates neocortical oscillations during NREM sleep." Nature communications 13.1 (2022): 5231.

12

Sterman, M. Bo, and CD13916976 Clemente. "Forebrain inhibitory mechanisms: sleep patterns induced by basal forebrain stimulation in the behaving cat." Experimental neurology 6.2 (1962): 103-117.

13

Penaloza-Rojas, J. H., M. Elterman, and N. Olmos. "Sleep induced by cortical stimulation." Experimental neurology 10.2 (1964): 140-147.

14

Akert, Konrad, W. P. Koella, and R. Hess Jr. "Sleep produced by electrical stimulation of the thalamus." American Journal of Physiology-Legacy Content 168.1 (1951): 260-267.

15

Shin, Anna, et al. "A brainstem-to-mediodorsal thalamic pathway mediates sound-induced arousal from slow-wave sleep." Current Biology 33.5 (2023): 875-885.

16

Bergeron, David, et al. "Central thalamic deep brain stimulation for disorders of consciousness: an individual participant data meta-analysis." Journal of Neurosurgery 1.aop (2025): 1-10.

17

Cain, Joshua A., et al. "Ultrasonic thalamic stimulation in chronic disorders of consciousness." Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation 14.2 (2021): 301-303.



Discuss

Fish AI Reader

Fish AI Reader

AI辅助创作,多种专业模板,深度分析,高质量内容生成。从观点提取到深度思考,FishAI为您提供全方位的创作支持。新版本引入自定义参数,让您的创作更加个性化和精准。

FishAI

FishAI

鱼阅,AI 时代的下一个智能信息助手,助你摆脱信息焦虑

联系邮箱 441953276@qq.com

相关标签

丘脑 意识状态 无意识 睡眠 脑功能
相关文章