少点错误 05月15日 17:52
What does it mean to "write like you talk"?
index_new5.html
../../../zaker_core/zaker_tpl_static/wap/tpl_guoji1.html

 

文章探讨了“写作如说话”这一常见建议的复杂性。Paul Graham认为书面语言比口语更差,因为它更复杂、正式和疏远。然而,Orwell指出,匆忙写作或口述时也容易陷入浮夸的文风。研究表明,口语和写作在从句使用、词汇密度和结构压缩等方面存在差异。口语更依赖情境和元话语,而写作更正式和精炼。因此,“写作如说话”的建议需要具体化,避免浮夸和冗长,追求清晰和简洁。

🗣️Paul Graham认为书面语言比口语更差,因为它更复杂、正式和疏远,导致读者注意力分散,并让作者产生虚假印象,误以为自己表达了更多内容。他建议通过大声朗读并修改来使写作更像对话。

✍️Orwell认为复杂的正式用语会掩盖空虚,用拉丁词汇堆砌事实,模糊细节。但他认为口语也可能因仓促而变得浮夸。与Graham不同,Orwell并未区分口语和书面语。

📊研究表明,口语和写作在从句使用上存在争议。一些研究发现口语包含更多从句,而另一些研究则发现写作更复杂。写作通常词汇密度更高,使用压缩风格,即使用名词短语而非从句来添加信息,这可能使关系变得模糊不清。

📢面对面交流使用更多“元话语”,以减轻说话者和听者的认知负荷。说话者会使用口头路标和支架来标记题外话,发出返回主线的信号,并保持整体结构在视图中。由于听众无法重读或依赖视觉提示,因此他们依赖这些标记以及关键点的重复和改述来保持方向,并知道何时将注意力转移回论证的中心线。

Published on May 15, 2025 9:49 AM GMT

People often say to “write like you talk.” Paul Graham has a post titled “Write Like You Talk” where he says explicitly that written language is worse than spoken language because

    “Written language is more complex, which makes it more work to read”“It’s also more formal and distant, which gives the reader’s attention permission to drift”“The complex sentences and fancy words give you, the writer, the false impression that you’re saying more than you actually are”

He gives concrete advice: “Before I publish a new essay, I read it out loud and fix everything that doesn't sound like conversation. … [If you have] writing so far removed from spoken language that it couldn't be fixed sentence by sentence … try explaining to a friend what you just wrote. Then replace the draft with what you said to your friend.”

In “Politics and the English Language,” Orwell also identifies complex and formal diction as a way to mask emptiness, “to dress up simple statements and give an air of scientific impartiality to biased judgements. … A mass of Latin words falls upon the facts like soft snow, blurring the outlines and covering up all the details. … If thought corrupts language, language can also corrupt thought.”

But Orwell doesn’t make the same distinction between spoken and written language; in fact he says that “When you are composing in a hurry—when you are dictating to a stenographer, for instance, or making a public speech—it is natural to fall into a pretentious, latinized style.” And Graham himself elsewhere says that “I'll often spend 2 weeks on an essay and reread drafts 50 times” and “I'm sure there are sentences I've read 100 times before publishing them” which is the opposite of how conversations work.

In contrast, Scott Alexander claimed that it only takes him “a couple of hours” to write a post and that “I don’t really understand why it takes so many people so long to write. They seem to be able to talk instantaneously, and writing isn’t that different from speech.” But then Scott is often considered a digressive or even “astoundingly verbose” writer.

There’s debate over whether speech or writing is more “complex” at all, with scholars taking sides based on the metrics they use for complexity and the datasets they analyze. In particular, there’s debate over whether speech or writing uses more subordinate clauses. Subordinate clauses are so called because they can’t stand as independent sentences. They have a few common types:

    The adverbial clause functions as an adverb: it modifies the sentence or some element of the sentence, e.g. by adding information about time, place, manner, reason, or condition.
      She arrived when the party had already started.After the rain stopped, we went for a walk.The game was canceled because of the heavy rain.
    The noun clause (or “nominal” or “content” clause) functions as a noun: usually it’s a that-clause or wh-clause.
      He told her (that) she was smart.I know what you did.She asked where the files were.
    The relative clause (or “adjectival” clause) functions like an adjective: it modifies a noun or pronoun.
      The book that I bought yesterday is excellent.Students who study regularly tend to perform better.The house where I grew up has been sold.

The most intuitive comparison of spoken and written English is between matched narratives; ask test subjects to describe the same scene with either an oral or written narrative. The two studies that use this method found more subordination in speech.

Other studies compared writing and conversation without any matched pairs:

But subordination isn’t the only measure of complexity. Consider the following two sentences:

The first is a single clause with no subordination while the second contains two nominal clauses: “living in the Gulf” and “living with oil” function as the subject and object respectively. But a reader would say that the first is more complex because it’s a longer sentence that uses longer words and abstract technical jargon. Studies consistently find that writing is more lexically dense, i.e. it has more words that convey content relative to grammatical or functional words.

For academic writing, the other major difference is that writing uses a compressed style that uses noun phrases rather than clauses to add information. These noun phrases often leave the underlying relationship implicit; even phrases like “heart disease” don’t reveal whether the meaning is “disease caused by the heart” or “disease located in the heart” or “disease affecting the heart” if readers don’t already know. Starting in the mid-20th century, multi-noun sequences like “air flow limitations” and “plasma concentration time curve” became more common in academic, newspaper, and medical prose. The relationships between these pre-modifying nouns is left implicit. The excerpt below is from the excellent Biber and Gray (2011).

The compressed style also often uses many layers of embedding in its noun phrases: consider “the effects [[of changes [in taxonomic resolution]][on analyses [of patterns [of multivariate variation [at different spatial scales]]]] [for the highly diverse fauna [inhabiting holdfasts [of the kelp Ecklonia radiata]]]].” So the main clause of a sentence can be very simple even when the phrasal modification is very complex: consider the sentence “This may indeed be part [of the reason [for the statistical link [between schizophrenia and membership [in the lower socioeconomic classes]]]].”

Biber and Gray (2011)

Lastly, a difference between speech and writing is that face-to-face conversation uses more “metadiscourse” to lighten the cognitive load for both the speaker and listener. The speaker moves from topic to topic on the fly based on what seems natural while using verbal signposts and scaffolding to mark digressions, signal a return to the main line of thought, and keep the overall structure in view. Because listeners can’t reread or rely on visual cues like headings and paragraph breaks, they depend on these markers—“to sum up,” “we’ll come back to that,” “by the way”—along with repetition and reformulation of key points to stay oriented and to know when to shift their attention back to the argument’s central thread.

So the common advice to "write like you talk" can be underspecified. It's good to avoid pretentious and formulaic cliches that mask the absence of precise thought, and separately to avoid dense and impenetrable jargon that's hard for non-experts to understand. But it's bad to write verbose and digressive meanderings without editing them. And because it’s faster to write, that kind of content can occupy a large share of posts (and more so of words) in Internet forums and discourse.



Discuss

Fish AI Reader

Fish AI Reader

AI辅助创作,多种专业模板,深度分析,高质量内容生成。从观点提取到深度思考,FishAI为您提供全方位的创作支持。新版本引入自定义参数,让您的创作更加个性化和精准。

FishAI

FishAI

鱼阅,AI 时代的下一个智能信息助手,助你摆脱信息焦虑

联系邮箱 441953276@qq.com

相关标签

写作技巧 口语 书面语 语言复杂性
相关文章